PL EN


Preferencje help
Widoczny [Schowaj] Abstrakt
Liczba wyników
2010 | 12 | 1 |

Tytuł artykułu

The vocal repertoire of two sympatric species of nectar-feeding bats (Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi)

Warianty tytułu

Języki publikacji

PL

Abstrakty

EN
We recorded social vocalizations from two sympatric species of glossophagine bats, Glossophaga soricina and G. commissarisi, using habituated captive groups that were housed in separate flight cages. Whenever possible, the species-specific vocalization types were described in the light of the social context in which they were produced. Several vocalization types within each species' repertoire had remarkable similarities to vocalization types in the other species' repertoire. Out of these, four vocalization types with interspecific acoustic similarities (approach pulses, distress calls, aggressive trills, and alert calls) were used in similar behavioral contexts in both species. Approach pulses were produced whenever a bat was flying towards an already occupied roost. Distress calls were uttered whenever a bat was attacked by conspecifics or restrained by the observer, whereas aggressive trills were produced during aggressive encounters prior to physical contact. Alert calls were uttered when bats were disturbed or when several individuals were circling the same location. The interspecific similarity of both the social context and the acoustic structure of vocalization types suggest that the potential for interspecific communication is high in G. soricina and G. commissarisi.

Wydawca

-

Rocznik

Tom

12

Numer

1

Opis fizyczny

p.205-215,fig.,ref.

Twórcy

  • Institute of Experimental Ecology, University of Ulm, Albert-Einstein Allee 11, 89069 Ulm, Germany
autor

Bibliografia

  • 1. J. Alvarez , M. R. Willig , J. K. Jones , and D. J. Webster . 1991. Glossophaga soricina. Mammalian Species, 379: 1–7. Google Scholar
  • 2. M. M. Andrews , P. T. Andrews , D. F. Wills , and S. M. Bevis . 2006. Ultrasound social calls of greater horseshoe bats (Rhinolophus ferrumequinum) in a hibernaculum. Acta Chiropterologica, 8: 197–212. Google Scholar
  • 3. T. Aubin 1991. Why do distress calls evoke interspecific responses? An experimental study applied to some species of birds. Behavioural Processes, 23: 103–111. Google Scholar
  • 4. J. R. Baker , J. K. Jones , and D. C. Carter . 1976. Biology of bats of the New World family Phyllostomatidae. Part I. Special Publications of the Museum of Texas Tech University, 10: 1–218. Google Scholar
  • 5. R. M. R. Barclay 1982. Interindividual use of echolocation calls: eavesdropping by bats. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 10: 271–275. Google Scholar
  • 6. R. M. R. Barclay , and D. W. Thomas . 1979. Copulation call of Myotis lucifugus: a discrete situation-specific communication signal. Journal of Mammalogy, 60: 632–634. Google Scholar
  • 7. R. M. R Barclay , M. B. Fenton , and D. W. Thomas . 1979. Social behavior of the little brown bat, Myotis lucifugus. 2. Vocal communication. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 6: 137–146. Google Scholar
  • 8. K. E. Barlow , and G. Jones . 1997. Function of pipistrelle social calls: field data and playback experiment. Animal Behaviour, 53: 991–999. Google Scholar
  • 9. A. Bastian , and S. Schmidt . 2008. Affect cues in vocalizations of the bat, Megaderma lyra, during agonistic interactions The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124: 598–608. Google Scholar
  • 10. O. Behr , and O. Von Helversen . 2004. Bat serenades — complex courtship songs of the sac-winged bat Saccopteryx bilineata. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 56: 106–115. Google Scholar
  • 11. K. M. Bohn , G. S. Wilkinson , and C. F. Moss . 2007. Discrimination of infant isolation calls by female greater spearnosed bats, Phyllostomus hastatus. Animal Behaviour, 73: 423–432. Google Scholar
  • 12. K. M. Bohn , B. Schmidt-French , S. T. Ma , G. D. Pollack . 2008a. Syllable acoustics, temporal patterns and call composition vary with behavioral context in Mexican free-tailed bats. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124: 1838–1848. Google Scholar
  • 13. K. M. Bohn , B. Schmidt-French , C. Schwartz , M. Smotherman , and G. D. Pollack . 2008b. Versatility and stereotypy of free-tailed bat songs. PLoS ONE 4(8): e6746. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006746. Google Scholar
  • 14. J. W. Boughman 1997. Greater spear-nosed bats give groupdistinctive calls. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 40: 61–70. Google Scholar
  • 15. J. W. Boughman 1998. Vocal learning by greater spear-nosed bats. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 265B: 227–233. Google Scholar
  • 16. J. W. Bradbury , and S. L. Vehrencamp . 1998. Principles of animal communication. Sinauer, Sunderland, MA, 882 pp. Google Scholar
  • 17. P. Brown 1976. Vocal communication in the pallid bat, Antrozous pallidus. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 41: 34–54. Google Scholar
  • 18. P. E. Brown , T. W. Brown , and A. D. Grinnell . 1983. Echolocation, development, and vocal communication in the lesser bulldog bat, Noctilio albiventris. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 13: 287–298. Google Scholar
  • 19. G. G. Carter , M. D. Skowronski , P. A. Faure , and M. B. Fenton . 2008. Antiphonal calling allows individual discrimination in white-winged vampire bats. Animal Behaviour, 76: 1343–1355. Google Scholar
  • 20. M. Chu 2001. Heterospecific responses to scream calls and vocal mimicry by phainopeplas (Phainopepla nitens) in distress. Behaviour, 138: 775–787. Google Scholar
  • 21. S. M. Davidson and G. S. Wilkinson . 2002. Geographic and individual variation in vocalizations by male Saccopteryx bilineata (Chiroptera: Emballonuridae). Journal of Mammalogy, 83: 526–535. Google Scholar
  • 22. K.-H. Esser , and U. Schmidt . 1989. Mother-infant communication in the lesser spear-nosed bat Phyllostomus discolor (Chiroptera, Phyllostomidae) — evidence for acoustic learning. Ethology, 82: 156–168. Google Scholar
  • 23. M. B. Fenton 1985. Communication in the Chiroptera. Indiana University Press, Bloomington, 176 pp. Google Scholar
  • 24. M. B. Fenton 2003. Eavesdropping on the echolocation and social calls of bats. Mammalian Review, 33: 193–204. Google Scholar
  • 25. M. B. Fenton , J. J. Belwood , J. H. Fullard , and T. H. Kunz . 1976. Responses of Myotis lucifugus (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae) to calls of conspecifics and to other sounds. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 54: 1443–1448. Google Scholar
  • 26. M. B. Fenton , E. Bernars , S. Bouchard , L. Hollis , D. S. Johnston , C. L. Lausen , J. M. Ratcliffe , D. K. Riskin , J. R. Taylor , and J. Zigouris . 2001. The bat fauna of Lamanai, Belize: roosts and trophic roles. Journal of Tropical Ecology, 17: 511–524. Google Scholar
  • 27. E. Fleming , T. Hooper , and D. E. Wilson . 1972. Three Central American bat communities: structure, reproductive cycles and movement patterns. Ecology, 53: 555–569. Google Scholar
  • 28. E. H. Gillam 2007. Eavesdropping by bats on the feeding buzzes of conspecifics. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 85: 795–801. Google Scholar
  • 29. G. G. Goodwin , and A. M. Greenhall . 1961. Review of the bats of Trinidad and Tobago. Bulletin of the American Museum of Natural History, 122: 187–302. Google Scholar
  • 30. G. L. Graham 1988. Interspecific associations among Peruvian bats at diurnal roosts and roost sites. Journal of Mammalogy, 69: 711–720. Google Scholar
  • 31. D. R. Griffin 1958. Listening in the dark. Yale University Press, New Haven, 413 pp. Google Scholar
  • 32. E. R. Hall , and W. W. Dalquest . 1963. The mammals of Veracruz. University of Kansas Publication of the Museum of Natural History, 14: 165–362. Google Scholar
  • 33. D. J. Howell 1983. Glossophaga soricina (murciélago lengualarga, nectar bat). Pp. 472–474, in Costa Rican natural history ( D. H. Janzen , ed.). University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 816 pp. Google Scholar
  • 34. H. Jahelková , I. Horáček , and T. Bartonička . 2008. The advertisement song of Pipistrellus nathusii (Chiroptera, Vespertilionidae): a complex message containing acoustic signatures of individuals. Acta Chiropterologica, 10: 103–126. Google Scholar
  • 35. S. Janben , and S. Schmidt . 2009. Evidence for a perception of prosodic cues in bat communication: contact call classification by Megaderma lyra. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, doi 10.1007/s00359-009-0441-8. Google Scholar
  • 36. M. A. Jurisevic , and K. J. A. Sanderson . 1998. A comparative analysis of distress call structure in Australian passerine and non-passerine species: influence of size and phylogeny. Journal of Avian Biology, 29: 61–71. Google Scholar
  • 37. J. S. Kanwal , S. Matsumura , K. Ohlemiller , and N. Suga . 1994. Analysis of acoustic elements and syntax in communication sounds emitted by mustached bats. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 96: 1229–1254. Google Scholar
  • 38. D. H. Kelm , K. R. Wiesner , and O. Von Helversen . 2008. Effects of artificial roosts for frugivorous bats on seed dispersal in a neotropical forest pasture mosaic. Conservation Biology, 22: 733–741. Google Scholar
  • 39. M. Knörnschild , and O. Von Helversen . 2008. Nonmutual vocal mother-pup recognition in the greater sac-winged bat. Animal Behavior, 76: 1001–1009. Google Scholar
  • 40. M. Knörnschild , O. Behr , and O. Von Helversen . 2006. Babbling behavior in the sac-winged bat (Saccopteryx bilineata). Naturwissenschaften, 93: 451–454. Google Scholar
  • 41. M. Knörnschild , O. Von Helversen , and F. Mayer . 2007. Twin siblings sound alike: individual variation in the noctule bat, Nyctalus noctula. Animal Behaviour, 74: 1055–1063. Google Scholar
  • 42. M. Knörnschild , M. Nagy , M. Metz , F. Mayer , and O. Von Helversen . 2009. Complex vocal imitation in a bat. Biology Letters, FirstCite Online, doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0685. Google Scholar
  • 43. R. K. Laval 1970. Banding returns and activity periods of some Costa Rican bats. Southwestern Naturalist, 15: 1–10. Google Scholar
  • 44. T. O. Lemke 1984. Foraging ecology of the long-nosed bat, Glossophaga soricina, with respect to resource availability. Ecology, 65: 538–548. Google Scholar
  • 45. J. Lopez , and C. Vaughan . 2007. Food niche overlap among Neotropical frugivorous bats in Costa Rica. Revista Biologia Tropical, 55: 301–313. Google Scholar
  • 46. J. Ma , K. Kobayasi , S. Zhang , and W. Metzner . 2006. Vocal communication in adult greater horseshoe bats, Rhinolophus ferrumequinum. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 192A: 535–550. Google Scholar
  • 47. R. D. Magarth , B. J. Pitcher , and J. L. Gardner . 2009. Recognition of other species' alarm calls: speaking the same language or learning another? Proceedings of the Royal Society London, 276B: 769–774. Google Scholar
  • 48. G. Pfalzer , and J. Kusch . 2003. Structure and variability of bat social calls: implications for specificity and individual recognition. Journal of Zoology (London), 261: 21–33. Google Scholar
  • 49. B. Pink 1996. Fortpflanzungs- und Sozialverhalten der blütenbesuchenden Fledermausart Glossophaga soricina (Phyllostomidae; Glossophaginae). M.Sc. Thesis, Department of Zoology, University of Erlangen-Nuremberg, Germany. Google Scholar
  • 50. J. M. Russ , and P. A. Racey . 2007. Species-specificity and individual variation in the song of male Nathusius' pipistrelles (Pipistrellus nathusii). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 61: 669–677. Google Scholar
  • 51. Russ, J. M. G. Jones , I. J. MacKie , and P. A. Racey . 2004. Interspecific responses to distress calls in bats (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae): a function for convergence in call design? Animal Behaviour, 67: 1005–1014. Google Scholar
  • 52. J. M. Russ , G. Jones , and P. A. Racey . 2005. Responses of soprano pipistrelles, Pipistrellus pygmaeus, to their experimentally modified distress calls. Animal Behaviour, 70: 397–404. Google Scholar
  • 53. M. J. Ryan , D. B. Clark , and J. A. Lackey . 1985. Response of Artibeus lituratus (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae) to distress calls of conspecifics. Journal of Mammalogy, 66: 179–181. Google Scholar
  • 54. A. A. Scherrer , and G. S. Wilkinson . 1993. Evening bat isolation calls provide evidence for heritable signatures. Animal Behaviour, 46: 847–860. Google Scholar
  • 55. R. Seyfarth , and D. Cheney . 1990. The assessment by vervet monkeys of their own and another species' alarm calls. Animal Behaviour, 40: 754–764. Google Scholar
  • 56. B. Siemers 2006. Bats: communication by ultrasound. Pp. 699–704, in The Encyclopedia of language and linguistics, 2nd edition ( K. Brown , ed.). Elsevier, Amsterdam, 9000 pp. Google Scholar
  • 57. R. Simon , M. W. Holderied , and O. Von Helversen . 2006. Size discrimination of hollow hemispheres by echolocation in a nectar-feeding bat. Journal of Experimental Biology, 209: 3599–3609. Google Scholar
  • 58. R. A. Stefanski , and J. B. Falls . 1972. A study of distress calls of song, swamp, and white-throated sparrows (Aves: Fringillidae). II. Interspecific responses and properties used in recognition. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 50: 1513–1525. Google Scholar
  • 59. M. Tschapka 2003. Pollination of the understorey palm Calyptrogyne ghiesbregthiane by hovering and perching bats. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 80: 281–288. Google Scholar
  • 60. M. Tschapka 2004. Energy density patterns of nectar resources permit coexistence within a guild of Neotropical flowervisiting bats. Journal of Zoology (London), 263: 7–21. Google Scholar
  • 61. D. Von Helversen 2004. Object classification by echolocation in nectar-feeding bats: size-independent generalization of shape. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 190A: 515–521. Google Scholar
  • 62. O. Von Helversen , and H.-U. Reyer . 1984. Nectar intake and energy expenditure in a flower visiting bat. Oecologia, 63: 178–184. Google Scholar
  • 63. D. Von Helversen , and O. Von Helversen . 2003. Object recognition by echolocation: a nectar-feeding bat exploiting the flowers of a rain forest vine. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 189A: 327–336. Google Scholar
  • 64. W. D. Webster , and J. Knox-Jones . 1993. Glossophaga commissarisi. Mammalian Species, 446: 1–4. Google Scholar
  • 65. R. H. Wiley , and D. G. Richards . 1982. Adaptations for acoustic communication in birds: sound propagation and signal detection. Pp. 131–181, in Acoustic communication in birds. Volume 1 ( D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller , eds.). Academic Press, New York, 360 pp. Google Scholar
  • 66. G. S. Wilkinson 1995. Information transfer in bats. Symposium of the Zoological Society of London, 67: 345–360. Google Scholar
  • 67. G. S. Wilkinson , and J. W. Boughman . 1998. Social calls coordinate foraging in greater spear-nosed bats. Animal Behaviour, 55: 337–350. Google Scholar
  • 68. M. Zagmajster 2003. Display song of parti-coloured bat Vespertilio murinus Linneus, 1758 (Chiroptera, Mammalia) in southern Slovenia and preliminary study of its variability. Natura Sloveniae, 5: 27–41. Google Scholar

Typ dokumentu

Bibliografia

Identyfikatory

Identyfikator YADDA

bwmeta1.element.dl-catalog-aef944b1-4e65-4510-adbb-4bab41acfc83
JavaScript jest wyłączony w Twojej przeglądarce internetowej. Włącz go, a następnie odśwież stronę, aby móc w pełni z niej korzystać.