PL EN


Preferencje help
Widoczny [Schowaj] Abstrakt
Liczba wyników

Czasopismo

2013 | 48 | 2 |

Tytuł artykułu

Parent recognition in chicks of the ground-nesting, nidifugous Saunders's Gull Saundersilarus saundersi

Warianty tytułu

PL
Rozpoznawanie rodziców przez pisklęta u mewy chińskiej

Języki publikacji

EN

Abstrakty

EN
In birds, offspring-parent interactions play an essential role in facilitating offspring survival at nest-leaving. In contrast to nidicolous nestlings, nidifugous chicks are expected to hatch in ground nests, leave the nest soon after hatching, and presumably exhibit precise parent-offspring recognition during this time. Yet, some studies document variation in nidifugous behavior and offspring-parent recognition of semi-precocial chicks during the nest-leaving stage in the family Laridae. We examine patterns in nest-leaving age, mobile capacity, and parent recognition in wild and captive chicks of the Saunders's Gull Saundersilarus saundersi that is one of ground nesting and colonially breeding species. Our results indicated that the development of locomotor activity in nidifugous chicks coincided with the nest- leaving age. Soliciting behavior of experimental chicks increased with age, but they strongly discriminated vocalizations of pseudo-parents against unfamiliar adults only at the early stage of nest-leaving. We suggest that parent recognition by chicks during this short period may play a key role in facilitating their own survival while moving to a new environment with their parents after leaving the nest. More studies using a comparative approach are needed to understand how nesting ecology, nest-leaving behavior, and parent-offspring interaction are interconnected in the members of the family Laridae.
PL
W pracy badano wiek, w którym pisklęta opuszczają gniazdo, ich mobilność w zależności od wieku oraz rozpoznawanie rodziców przez pisklęta u mewy chińskiej. Badania prowadzono zarówno w terenie, jak i w warunkach laboratoryjnych prowadząc obserwacje sztucznie wychowywanych piskląt. Pisklęta ze sztucznego wychowu, od dnia wyklucia do trzeciego dnia życia, zapoznawano z nagranym głosem osobnika dorosłego (głos pseudorodzicielski). Następnie, gdy były one w wieku 5-6, 10-11 i 15-16 dni badano ich reakcję na głos pseudorodzicielski oraz głos innego, nieznanego osobnika dorosłego. Stwierdzono, że w naturze pisklęta w wieku do trzech dni są jeszcze obserwowane w okolicy gniazda, a odległość ich przebywania od gniazda wzrasta wraz z wiekiem (Fig. 1). U sztucznie wychowywanych piskląt mobilność wzrastała wraz z wiekiem, szczególnie silnie pomiędzy drugim i czwartym dniem życia (Fig. 2). Reakcja piskląt na głos dorosłego osobnika wzrastała z ich wiekiem oraz była zależna od rodzaju emitowanego głosu. Ponadto pisklęta w wieku 5-6 dni wyraźnie silniej niż w wieku 10-11 i 15-16 dni reagowały na głosy pseudorodzicielskie w porównaniu z głosem nieznanego osobnika (Fig. 3, 4, Tab. 1).

Słowa kluczowe

Wydawca

-

Czasopismo

Rocznik

Tom

48

Numer

2

Opis fizyczny

p.245-252,fig.,ref.

Twórcy

autor
  • Ecological Institute for Oriental Stork, Korea National University of Education, Cheongwon 363-791, Republic of Korea
autor
  • Department of Biology Education, Korea National University of Education, Cheongwon 363-791, Republic of Korea
autor
  • Department of Biology Education, Korea National University of Education, Cheongwon 363-791, Republic of Korea
autor
  • Department of Biology Education, Korea National University of Education, Cheongwon 363-791, Republic of Korea
autor
  • Department of Biology Education, Korea National University of Education, Cheongwon 363-791, Republic of Korea
  • Ecological Institute for Oriental Stork, Korea National University of Education, Cheongwon 363-791, Republic of Korea

Bibliografia

  • Aubin T., Jouventin P. 2002. How to vocally identify kin in a crowd: the penguin model. Adv. Stud. Behav. 31: 243-277.
  • Beecher M. D., Beecher I. M., Hahn S. 1981. Parent-offspring recognition in bank swallows (Riparia riparia): II. Development and acoustic basis. Anim. Behav. 29: 95-101.
  • Beecher M. D., Stoddard P. K., Loesche P. 1985. Recognition of parent's voices by young cliff swallows. Auk 102: 600-605.
  • Besnard A., Gimenez O., Lebreton J. 2002. A model for the evolution of crèching behaviour in gulls. Evol. Ecol. 16: 489-503.
  • Besnard A., Sadoul N., Lebreton J. 2006. First quantitative comparison of aggression between crèching and non-crèching Larid species. Waterbirds 29: 481-488.
  • Bolhuis J. J. 1991. Mechanisms of avian imprinting: a review. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 66: 303-345.
  • Burtt E. H. 1977. Some factors in the timing of parent-chick recognition in swallows. Anim. Behav. 25: 231-239.
  • Cam E., Monnat J.-Y., Hines J. E. 2003. Long-term fitness consequences of early conditions in the kittiwake. J. Anim. Ecol. 72: 411-424.
  • Charrier I., Mathevon N., Jouventin P. 2002. How does a fur seal mother recognize the voice of her pup? An experimental study of Arctocephalus tropicalis. J. Exp. Biol. 205: 603-612.
  • Clutton-Brock T. H. 1991. The evolution of parental care. Princeton University Press, Princeton.
  • Davies S. J. J. F., Carrick R. 1962. On the ability of crested terns, Sterna bergii, to recognize their own chicks. Aust. J. Zool. 10: 171-177.
  • Glück E. 1984. Habitat selection in birds and the role of early experience. Z. Tierpsychol. 55: 45-54.
  • del Hoyo J., Elliott A., Sargatal J. 1996. Handbook of the Birds of the World. Vol. 3. Lynx Edicions, Barcelona.
  • IUCN 2013. IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version 2013.1. www.iucnredlist.org., accessed on 1 Nov. 2013.
  • Jacot A., Reers H., Forstmeier W. 2010. Individual recognition and potential recognition errors in parent-offspring communication. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 64: 1515-1525.
  • Kwon Y. S., Chung H. 2009. Breeding status and ecology of Saunders' gulls (Larus saundersi) in Songdo reclaimed land, west coast of Korea. Ocean Polar Res. 31: 277-282.
  • Leonard M. L., Horn A. G., Brown C. R., Fernandez N. J. 1997. Parent-offspring recognition in tree swallows, Tachycineta bicolor. Anim. Behav. 54: 1107-1116.
  • Martin T. E. 1995. Avian life history evolution in relation to nest sites, nest predation, and food. Ecol. Monogr. 65: 101-127.
  • Mathevon N., Charrier I., Jouventin P. 2003. Potential for individual recognition in acoustic signals: a comparative study of two gulls with different nesting patterns. C. R. Biol. 326: 329-337.
  • McGregor P. K. 1992. Quantifying responses to playback: one, many, or composite multivariate measures. In: McGregor P. K. (ed.). Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum, New York, pp. 79-96.
  • McGregor P. K., Catchpole C. K., Dabelsteen T., Falls J. B., et al. 1992. Design of playback experiments: the Thornbridge Hall NATO ARW consensus. In: McGregor P K. (ed.). Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum, New York, pp. 1-9.
  • Mennill D. J., Ratcliffe L. M. 2000. A field test of 'Syrinx' sound analysis software in interactive playback. Bioacoustics 11: 77-86.
  • Mulard H., Aubin T., White J. F., Hatch S. A., Danchin É. 2008. Experimental evidence of vocal recognition in young and adult black-legged kittiwakes. Anim. Behav. 76: 1855-1861.
  • Mulard H., Danchin É. 2008. The role of parent-offspring interactions during and after fledging in the black-legged kittiwake. Behav. Processes 79: 1-6.
  • Nice M. M. 1962. Development of behavior in precocial birds. Trans. Linn. Soc. NY. 8: 1-211.
  • Park H. W. 2003. Breeding ecology of Saunders' gulls (Larus saundersi Swinhoe) in Korea and recommendations for their conservation. Ph.D. Thesis. Korea National University of Education.
  • Park H. W. 2010. Breeding populations trend of the Saunders' gull (Larus saundersi Swinhoe) in Incheon Bay. Korean J. Environ. Biol. 28: 49-55.
  • Park S. R., Chung H. 2002. How do young black-tailed gulls (Larus crassirostris) recognize adult voice signals? Korean J. Biol. Sci. 6: 221-225.
  • Parker N., Cam E., Lank D. B., Cooke F. 2003. Post-fledging survival of marbled murrelets Brachyramphus marmoratus estimated with radio-marked juveniles in Desolation Sound, British Columbia. Mar. Ornithol. 31: 207-212.
  • Riedman M. L. 1982. The evolution of alloparental care and adoption in mammals and birds. Q. Rev. Biol. 57: 405-435.
  • Riou S., Chastel O., Hamer K. C. 2012. Parent-offspring conflict during the transition to independence in a pelagic seabird. Behav. Ecol. 23: 1102-1107.
  • Searby A., Jouventin P., Aubin T. 1997. Acoustic recognition in macaroni penguins: an original signature system. Anim. Behav. 67: 615-625.
  • Shy M. M. 1982. Interspecific feeding among birds — a review. J. Field Ornithol. 53: 370-393.
  • Skutch A. F. 1976. Parent birds and their young. University of Texas Press, Austin.
  • Starck J. M. 1993. Evolution of avian ontogenies. In: Powers D. M. (ed.). Curr. Ornithol. Plenum Press, 10: 275-366.
  • Starck J. M., Ricklefs R. E. 1998. Avian growth and development: evolution within the altricial-precocial spectrum. Oxford University Press, New York.
  • Storey A. E., Anderson R. E., Porter J. M., Maccharles A. M. 1992. Absence of parent-young recognition in kittiwakes: a re-examination. Behaviour 120: 302-323.
  • Yoon J., Lee S. H., Joo E. J., Na K. J., Park S. R. 2013. Sexual differences in post-hatching Saunders's gulls: size, locomotor activity, and foraging skill. Zool. Sci. 30: 262-266.

Typ dokumentu

Bibliografia

Identyfikatory

Identyfikator YADDA

bwmeta1.element.agro-16221d3c-903c-40da-a90f-6733be71080d
JavaScript jest wyłączony w Twojej przeglądarce internetowej. Włącz go, a następnie odśwież stronę, aby móc w pełni z niej korzystać.